THE POSITION OF THE REJANG LANGUAGE OF SUMATRA IN RELATION TO MALAY AND THE `ABLAUT' LANGUAGES OF NORTHWEST BORNEO
0. Introduction
This paper suggests that certain findings with respect to the historical phonology of Rejang can shed light on similar developments in the histories of Malay and the `ablaut' languages of Northwest Borneo. The argument begins with a top‑down analysis of sound changes in Rejang, with special attention to the role of prosodic structure, especially word-level stress. In McGinn (1997) it was claimed that Rejang underwent not just one but two prosodic changes (stress shifts), one paralleled by (and conceivably shared with) Malay, and a second placing the stress uniformly on the final syllable of the word. If accepted, this claim raises at least the following questions.
(1) (i) How many languages besides Rejang underwent the FIRST
stress shift parallel to (or possibly together with)
Malay?
(ii) Are there any related languages besides Rejang showing
evidence of multiple prosodic changes?
(iii) Is there any other (segmental) evidence supporting a
lower-order subgroup for Rejang?
Rejang assistants were Dr. Zainubi Arbi and Arma Zuazla (Musi dialect), Irlan Caya (Kebanagung) and Sabidin Ishak (Lebong). Invaluable help was given by Amran Halim and Zainab Bakir. Helpful comments and suggestions provided by Robert Blust and two ananymous reviewers. All are hereby gratefully acknowledged.
This paper begins to address questions (i)-(ii) and explores some possible implications with respect to (iii). As an example of the latter, strict adherence to the "family tree" model of linguistic differentiation precludes any close relationship between Rejang, Malay, and the languages of Borneo, despite numerous similarities in their historical developments. In particular, identical changes that might possibly support a hypothesis linking Rejang with Malay in a lower-order subgroup are undermined by independent changes in Rejang that cannot be reconciled with any subgrouping hypothesis. See especially changes A-(1)-(4) in Appendix A. Similar remarks apply to a chain of languages and dialects in Northwest Borneo reported in Robert Blust (1997). Thus, both the fact of Rejang's geographical isolation and the family tree theory applied strictly to the comparative data converge to suggest that Rejang developed independently in highland Southwest Sumatra, and, although surrounded by Malay dialects and penetrated to the bone by the national language (Indonesian-Malay), and although sharing many features with certain Borneo languages, Rejang nonetheless belongs to no linguistic subgroup lower than Malayo-Polynesian. What makes this conclusion more interesting than it might otherwise be is the range of parallel developments (parallel drifts[1]) in Rejang, Malay, and the `ablaut' languages of Northwest Borneo in modern-day Sarawak, Malaysia. Careful documentation of parallel drifts contributes to the typological study of sound change.
1.0 Methodological Preliminaries
In both phonology and morpho-syntax, Rejang displays some Malay-like and some `un-Malay' historical drifts. The first set is reviewed in the next subsection below; the second (much larger) set is discussed beginning with section 2.1.
1.1 Four Malay-like Morpho-syntactic Drifts
Many if not all of the Malay-like tendencies are older than the non-Malay drifts, and were conditioned (in part) by the FIRST STRESS SHIFT. This statement seems valid for morpho-syntax as well as for phonology, given the obvious proviso that certain similarities are due to common inheritance from PAN/PMP. Consider the following four candidates.
(2) a. Word Order Change from VS to SV (cf. Blust 1997:16)
b. Loss of PAN `focus' suffixes *-en and *-an[2]
c. Re-analysis of PAN `perfective' infix *-in- as the
passive marker
d. Loss of PAN "focus" prefix affix *Si-
Rejang, Malay, and the ablaut languages of Sarawak all presumably underwent similar grammatical changes whereby the PAN four-focus voice system (Philippine-type) became a morphologically simpler active-passive voice system (Dahl 1976; Blust 1997:17). Although the correct theoretical characterization of either system is far from settled, the morphological changes (2c-d) are correlated with the word order change (2a) and (ex hypothesi) the FIRST STRESS SHIFT (to penult stress) put forth in McGinn (1997) and this paper (see next section). Finally, the reanalysis change (2c) was clearly `compensatory' in relation to the loss of suffixes (2b): the passive-like functions of *-en and *-an were re-assigned to the perfective infix *-in-, and the perfective function became secondary or disappeared categorically. This explains why reflexes of PAN/PMP *-in- serve as the passive marker in all three languages compared in this paper. Consider the display in (3).
(3) Contemporary Reflexes of PAN/PMP *ni- ~ *-in-
Malay di- (di-tembak `be shot')[3]
Rejang ne- ~ n- ~ -en- (t-en-ia' `be shot')
Mukah Melanau ne- ~ n- ~ -en- ~ -i- (bibed `be tied')
In the next sub-section, I will present four hypotheses relating to prosodic change.
1.2 Hypothesis: Two Prosodic Changes, Two Typologies
My major claims can be stated in the form of four hypotheses relating to Rejang, Malay, and the `ablaut' languages of coastal Sarawak. First, I assume that all these languages underwent the same (or very similar) prosodic change, conceivably as a shared innovation but more likely as parallel development, whereby the word-level accent (hereafter, simply the stress) shifted to the penult in a Malay-type pattern[4]. See Appendix C for the Rejang evidence. (In the Malay-type pattern the stress falls on the ultimate when the penult is schwa; otherwise on the penult.) Second, Rejang and the ablaut languages of Sarawak, but not Malay, underwent a Second Stress Shift whereby the stress was placed uniformly on the final syllable of the (phonological) word. See section 2.1. Third, between these two prosodic changes, when all these languages had the Malay-type stress pattern, the stress pattern conditioned the morphosyntactic changes mentioned in (2) above. Fourth, and most significantly, in phonology the Maay-type stress pattern conditioned the reduction of certain trisyllables to disyllables. This last included no fewer than five segmental innovations, all of which occurred in the same chronological order in the affected languages, as follows.
(4) "Blust's Law" (BL):
I. Prepenultimate *a Neutralization (PN *a)
II. Prepenultimate *#e- Deletion (PD *e)
III. Schwa syncope (SS)
IV. Intervocalic -CC- Reduction (CR)
V. Prepenultimate *i, *u Neutralization (PN *i,*u)
This set shall be referred to in this paper as "Blust's Law" (BL).
First, a caveat: Schwa Syncope (SS) is extremely widespread in Western Austronesian languages, and of itself may have little subgrouping value; what is significant is the interaction of SS with PN and CR. Originally discovered in the historical phonology of Malay (Blust 1982), the same rules (in the same order) were reported for the `ablaut' languages of Sarawak (Blust 1997). Thus: "There is some evidence that the first prepenultimate vowel to merge with schwa was *a, followed by *u and lastly *i" in the Mukah Melanau language of Sarawak (1997:21). This statement characterizes the effect of BL and explains the retention of prepenultimate PMP *i as /i/ and *u as /u/ (or /o/) in original trisyllables. Compare PMP *timeRaq > *timRaq > *timaq > Rejang timea' = Malay timah `tin'); PMP *tuqelang > Malay and Mukah /tulang/ (but Rejang /telan/ `bone' owing to the effect of rule A-(1) of Appendix A).
Blust's Law interacted with other changes causing losses of whole prepenultimate syllables. Thus loss of *q- (Rejang change A-(2) in Appendix A) interacted with loss of *a through PN and PD, causing loss of initial sequence *qa-, e.g. *qapeju > pegew `gall'. Compare this effect with the following statement by Blust: "All 40 or more languages of Borneo have lost initial *a, or *a preceded by *q or *S (which disappeared) in prepenultimate position, thereby reducing a number of original trisyllables to disyllables. A similar change has taken place in Malay and some other western MP languages." (Blust 1990b:240). Such changes contributed to what Blust (1990:244) has characterized as "the widespread disyllabic canonical target of Austronesian languages". This statement is obviously valid for Rejang as well. See Appendix A.
In morphology, notice that the FIRST STRESS SHIFT entails that the stress never fell on affixes[5]. Blust hints at a similar idea in the following remark: "It is possible that (the) drift-like tendency to lose pretonic vocalic distinctions initiated the transformation of the Proto-Austronesian `focus' system in western Indonesia through eliminating the instrumental marker *Si-." (Blust 1990b:240). To account for the other morpho-syntactic changes in (2), equally powerful drifts must be recognized; and they, too, seem to be motivated (in part at least) in terms of the stress pattern.
1.3. Syllable-Reduction Schemata Interpreted By the Family Tree Model
It appears that the earliest segmental changes in Rejang's independent history, especially B-(1) but also B-(2)-(4) in Appendix B, were chronologically prior to the BL set shown in (4) above (cf. B-(5)-(9) in Appendix B). In particular, change B-(1) was, apparently, unique to Rejang, which then `fed' B-(2)-(4). This sequence suffices to undermine any subgrouping arguments within the family tree theory. (See Section 6 for an alternative interpretation of the facts.)
2.1 Extensions of "Blust's Law" Across Morpheme Boundaries
The remaining comparisons involve relatively recent changes in Rejang and Mukah Melanau, and differ from the above in that Malay fades into the background. The evidence suggests that Rejang is typologically closer to the Sarawakan languages than to Malay. This claim is consistent with my hypothesis that Rejang and the Sarawakan languages underwent a SECOND STRESS SHIFT independently, whereby the stress fell on the ultimate. The hypothesis accounts directly for the fact that in each of the contemporary languages under consideration here, excepting of course Malay, the stress falls upon the final syllable of the (phonological) word. It also accounts for numerous parallel developments unattested in Malay and linking Rejang typologically with the coastal Sarawak group.
2.1 Mukah Melanau Simple Ablaut
In Rejang and the Sarawakan languages (but not Malay), "Blust's Law" applied across morhpeme boundaries, with morphological consequences. In Borneo simple ablaut arose via straightforward historical extension of SS and CR before PN (*i,*u) across morpheme boundaries. By contrast, over in Sumatra the re-application of BL changes PN (*i, *u) and SS was in reverse order in (infixed) words, resulting in word-medial consonant clusters. Consider the comparisons in Table 1.
Mukah seput `blowpipe' Rejang tengoa `hear'
PMP *s-um-eput, *s-in-eput *d-um-engeR, *d-in-engeR
SS CumCVC CinCVC
CR CuCVC CiCVC
PN(*i,*u) CemeCVC CeneCVC
SS CemCVC CenCVC
Outcome suput siput temngoa tenngoa
Table 1: Infixation of CeCVC Bases
From the display in Table 1 we can derive an explanation for why certain Rejang and Mukah affixed verbs are disyllables (and others remain trisyllables). In both languages, a selection from the set of BL changes re-applied affected infixed verbs across morpheme boundaries. The affected bases were `oxytone' (CeCVC)[6].
Despite the differences in morphological outcomes, it is important to bear in mind that Rejang bases underwent the BL changes in the same chronological order as Malay and Mukah Melanau bases. See rules A-(5)-(9) in Appendix A and Blust (1982, 1997).
2.2 Mukah Melanau "Compound Ablaut"
Vowel-initial bases increased their frequency in both Rejang and Mukah Melanau owing to the loss of base-initial *p- and *b- in transitive verbs. Although the mechanisms differed in detail, this parallel is perhaps the most striking of all. In Mukah the effect was what Blust calls compound ablaut. Like simple ablaut, compound ablaut involves infixed bases of the shape CeCVC[7]; what distinguishes the latter is that the initial C- is labial (*p or *b)[8]. In other words, in Mukah CeCVC bases lost the labial as part of the historical process that gave rise to compound ablaut. Schematically:
(5) BASE WORD PMP
Mukah compound ablaut: *bebed mubed < *b-um-ebed (active) Mukah simple ablaut:: *bebed bibed < *b-in-ebed (passive) Compound ablaut is relevant for active voice. To account for it, Blust offers an ordered set of changes that began with Infix Metathesis (IM) followed by the familiar BL changes. (6) *b-um-ebed > mu-bebed (IM) > mubbed (SS) > mubed (CR).
*p-um-epek > mu-pepek (IM) > muppek (SS) > mupek (CR)
2.3 Loss of *p... and *b... in Rejang Transitive Verbs
The Rejang changes paralleling the development of compound ablaut in Mukah were less radical morphologically, but more far-reaching in terms of the impact on the lexicon. Consider the following Rejang data.
(7) BASE WORD PMP Gloss
onoa' monoa' *b-um-unuq `kill'
onoa' nonoa' *b-in-unuq `be killed'
gong megong [m.gong] *p-um-egeng `hold'
gong negong [n.gong] *p-in-egeng 'be held'
In Rejang, prepenultimate infix vowels became schwa by extension of BL), and then word-initial *pe... and *be... simply disappeared[9]. The effect was an increase in the frequency of vowel-initial bases, and therefore of single-phoneme alternants /m-, n-, k- and p-/, giving rise to morphologically complex disyllables like m-onoa', negong, me-lié; ne-lié `give; be given' (<> *me-, *ne-) when added to vowel-initial bases, motivated by a phonotactic constraint (not found in Malay) disallowing VV sequences when the first V is schwa; and Mukah developed a fourth type alternant (ablaut) as previously discussed.
In the remainder of this section are listed a number of morphological features of Rejang that are either (virtual) retentions from PAN/PMP, or are derived from PAN/PMP by the application of one or a chain of rules beginning with the set labeled "Blust's Law" (BL). For data displays below, the following symbols have been adopted from Blust (1997) to facilitate comparison with Mukah: (B = Base; S = Stem; A = Active; P = Passive; N = Nominal; NS = Nasal Substitution).
1. Rejang has two (or possibly three) active-voice prefixes which distinguish degree of transitivity: the alternating prefix/infix me- ~ m- ~ -em-; and the doublet meng- ~ nge- that triggers `Nasal Substitution'.
(9) B: tebas
A: t-em-bas `to clear-cut'
A: menebas `clear-cutting' meng-t... > men-t > men-
A: nebas `clear-cutting' nge-t... > n-t > n-
P: t-en-bas `be clear-cut'
2. Owing to phonologically conditioned complementation among affixes, the phoneme /n/ is free to serve as an active or a passive prefix, depending on the lexical nature of the verb. Compare /nebas/ (active) above and /nonoa'/ (passive) below.
(10) B: onoa'
A: monoa' `kill'
P: nonoa'
3. The prefix meng- surfaces as /meng/- before bases that begin with a vowel, but another prefix ke- is sometimes added to bases that begin with /l/-.
(11) B: léa' `see'
S: ke-léa' 'see' (imperative)
A: k-em-léa' `see'
A: mengeléa' `observe' (meng+ke+léa')
P: k-en-kéa' `be seen'
An exception is me-leket `stick, adhere' (not **mengeleket). Rejang me-leket derives from PMP *mang+deket.
4. A morphophonemic extension of Schwa Syncope is a synchronic rule affecting infixed bases that begin with a consonant followed by schwa. (See change III of Blust's Law; Table 1 above; and rule B-(7) of Appendix B.)
(12) B: teko `come'
A: t-em-eko > temko `cause to come; arrange to bring s.o. in'
P: t-en-eko > tenko `be caused to come'
5. Historically, base-initial labial consonants (/p/-, /b/-, /m/-) disappeared in transitive verbs.
(13)
PAN B A P GLOSS
*balik bélék -- --- return (intr)
*b-um-unu qonoa' monoa' nonoa' kill
*p-um-injem injem minjem ninjem borrow
*p-um-egeng gong megong negong hold
As mentioned in section 2.2 above, the conditions closely resemble the conditions for compound ablaut in Mukah Melanau as described by Blust (1997:15).
6. Also paralleled in Mukah Melanau (cf. Blust (1997:22) is a strong tendency to develop vowel-initial verb bases by analogical back-formation based on the affixational pattern resulting from the above change.
(14) B: ékér
N: pékér `thought' (Arabic fikir `think' (Malay pikir))
A: mékér `think'
P: nékér `be thought',`thought s/he' (in narratives)
(15) B: teret
N: pe-teret `photograph' (English: portrait)
A: t-em-ret `take (picture)'
P: t-en-ret `(picture) be taken'
Given that m(e)-, n(e)-, p(e)- exist as prefixes, analogical back-formation tends to convert borrowed initial labials /p..., b..., m.../ into prefixes whenever the semantics allows[11].
4.0 Phonological Changes Associated With Second Stress Shift
Rejang and Mukah Melanau underwent at least two changes typical of languages with word-final stress: diphthongization of final high vowels, and weakening or loss of intervocalic consonant clusters. As illustrated in Appendix B, stressed *-i and *-u
diphthongized In Rejang.
(16) *is:i > ise:y `contents'
*ulu: > ule:w `head'
According to Blust, "In other languages which diphthongize *-i and *-u the stress pattern is oxytone: Chamic, Mukah, and other Melanau dialects of coastal Sarawak." (personal communication, April 1995). Second, compare PMP Nasal Cluster Reduction in Rejang and Mukah. In Rejang two classes of phonemes developed from intervocalic sequences of (homorganic) nasal + obstruent.
(17) *sempit > sepit [spit] `narrow'
*tungked > tokot [to.ko:t] `staff, cane'
*timbak > tia' [ti.a:'] `shoot'
*induk > io' [i.o:'] `mother (of animals)'
In Rejang, the nasal was dropped when the stop was voiceless (*mp > p, *nt > t, etc.); but when the stop was voiced the sequence coalesced into a `barred nasal' (*mb > , *nt > , etc.)[12]. In Mukah the nasal was simply dropped (Blust 1997:20). Thus in both languages all PMP homorganic nasal clusters became single phonemes.
Finally, Rejang probably represents the extreme for an Austronesian language in terms of the number of vocalic changes in its phonological history (Blust 1984), including harmonization of base vowels, e.g. *langit > léngét `sky'; *qutek > oto' `brain', *sapu (> *supu) > supew `broom'; *tali > (*tili) > tiley `rope'. See Appendix C rules (1)-(2).
5. Summary
McGinn (1997) argued that two prosodic changes conditioned most of the regular segmental shifts in Rejang's historical phonology. In this paper, it has been suggested that comparable prosodic changes may have occurred in the `ablaut' languages of Northwest Borneo as described by Blust (1997). The FIRST STRESS SHIFT, defined in terms of prosodic change to a Malay-type stress pattern, may account for the fact that the stress is never assigned initially to an affix in Rejang, Malay, and the ablaut languages of Northwest Borneo. An unusually strong historical tendency to develop binary (disyllabic) BASES has been linked (ex hypothesi) to this aspect of the stress pattern and to a set of five syllable-reduction rules labled "Blust's Law" (BL). A subset of the affected languages, exemplified by Malay, retains the older stress pattern; furthermore, contemporary Malay favors trisyllabic (and above) affixed words, and the stress alternates under conditions of suffixation[13]. The second (newer) stress pattern is represented by Rejang and the Bornean languages. This subset underwent a second prosodic change whereby the stress came to fall uniformly on the final syllable of the word; medial consonalt clusters were simplified; and final high vowels were diphthongized. Moreover, the set of changes labeled "Blust's Law" applied across morpheme boundaries, causing the phonological reduction of certain affixed verbs to disyllables. In this way, the preference for binaryness extended beyond wordbases and invaded the domain of the PHONOLOGICAL WORD. In Mukah this preference was implemented by ablaut (CuiCVC); in Rejang by CemnCVC formations. In Rejang at least, the pattern is found only in morphologically complex verbs (never in simple bases)[14]. There are no suffixes in Rejang, and as a direct consequence alternating base-stress is an alien concept.
6. Theoretical Conclusions
It is always a mistake to use Sapir's singulary term `drift' to refer to a plurality of parallel developments in daughter speech communities following dialect- or language-split. There is no singulary theory of `drift', but only theories of linguistic change; and linguistic change can be implemented within the family tree model or the wave model. By contrast, the phrase `parallel drift(s)' can represent a valid concept when used as a near-synonym for `parallel changes' or `parallel developments'; the metaphor of `drift' adds to these the expectation that linguistic changes are never totally isolated events, but always in some sense `directional' vis-a vis other changes. Furthermore, one subset of linguistic changes, namely sound changes, tend toward absolute regularity under conditions of reconstruction; and yet they cannot be predicted in real time. It follows that after language split, daughter languages should (and apparently always do) develop individually, yet in many parallel ways; unpredictably, yet vaguely in the same direction, as determined by earlier changes.
The family tree model applied strictly to the comparative data indicates that the earliest changes affecting Rejang as an independent language (with the possible exception of FIRST STRESS SHIFT) must be ordered after split. These are the earliest changes, namely, A-(1)-(4) in Appendix A. If so, then as mentioned in section 1.3, the similarities noted in this paper cannot be interpreted as shared innovations but must be either retentions from the protolanguage or parallel developments (=parallel drifts).
There is another interpretation that might be given, however, and it is provided by the wave model. Suppose contemporary Rejang, Malay and the ablaut languages of Borneo, all began as slightly divergent dialects of a single language occupying a single geographic area (such as Northwest Borneo), and that there was considerable areal influence, before one of the dialects migrated to southern Sumatra. Within this scenario, each language would be expected to show one or more unique changes while sharing others that had spread throughout the entire homeland area as shared innovations. The BL changes offer the most obvious candidates for a set of shared innovations; and Rejang changes (1)-(4) in Appendix A are candidates for pre-split changes that failed to spread to other dialects. Finally, the wave theory offers the possibility of adding Rejang changes A-(9)-(15) of Appendix A, and the SECOND STRESS SHIFT, to the set of pre-split changes which spread to some dialects but not others (certainly not to pre-Malay). Be that as it may, there is no doubt that the pre-Rejang dialect group migrated from its point of origin, wherever it was. If it was indeed coastal Sarawak, then the outgroup would have had a relatively easy sail along the west coast of Borneo and across a hundred miles of open sea to Sumatra, and thence to Rejang country, passing either by way of Bangka Island and up the Musi and Rawas rivers to the fertile foothills of the Barisan mountains and the contemporary Lebong-Rawas dialect area in present-day South Sumatra, or by way of the Sunda strait and along the west coast of Sumatra to the contemporary Pasisir dialect area in present-day Bengkulu.
REFERENCES
Adams, Karen L. and Thomas John Hudak, eds. 1992. Papers from the
Second Annual Meeting of the Southeast Asian Linguistic
Society. Tempe, AZ: Arizona State University Program for
Southeast Asian Studies.
Baldi, Phillip, ed. 1990. Trends in Linguistics: Studies and
Monographs 45. Berlin: Mouton.
Blust, Robert A. 1982. An overlooked feature of Malay historical
phonology. Bulletin of the School of Oriental and African
Studies, Vol. XLV.2:284-299.
. 1984. On the history of the Rejang vowels and
diphthongs. BTLV 140:422-450.
. 1990a. Patterns of sound change in the Austronesian
languages. In Philip Baldi, ed., pp. 231-267.
. 1990b. Linguistic change and reconstruction methodology
in the Austronesian language family. In Philip Baldi, ed.,
pp. 133-153.
, ed. 1991. Currents in Pacific Linguistics: Papers in
Honor of George W. Grace. Pacific Linguistics C-117.
. 1992. The Austronesian settlement of mainland Southeast
Asia. In Adams and Hudak, eds., pp. 25-83.
. 1997. Ablaut in Northwest Borneo. Diachronica XIV:1-30.
Coady, James and Richard McGinn. On the so-called implosive nasals
of Rejang. GAVA' 17:437-449.
Cohn, Abigail C. and John J. McCarthy. 1994 MS. Alignment and
parallelism in Indonesian prosody.
Dahl, Otto Christian. 1976. Proto-Austronesian. Lund: Scandinavian Institute of Asian
Studies Monograph Series 15.
McGinn, Richard. 1997. Some irregular reflexes of Proto-Malayo- Polynesian vowels in
the Rejang language of Sumatra. Diachronica XIV.66-107.
Ross, Malcom. 1992. The sound of Proto-Austronesian: an
outsider's view. Oceanic Linguistics 31.1:23-64.
Teeuw, A. 1959. The history of the Malay language. BTLV.115:138-
156.
Wolff, John. 1981. Similarities between Indonesian and Tagalog
and their historical basis. Historical Linguistics in
Indonesia, Part I, ed. by Robert A. Blust. NUSA 10:83-90.
1991. The PAN Phoneme *t and the grouping of the
Austronesian languages. In Robert Blust, ed., pp 535-549.
Zorc, R. David. 1983. Proto-Austronesian accent revisited.
Philippine Journal of Linguistics 14.1:1-24.
. 1991 MS. Austronesian and Philippine accent
patterns. Paper delivered at the Sixth International
Conference for Austronesian Linguistics, Honolulu.
. 1995. A glossary of Austronesian reconstructions. In
Daryl Tryon, ed. Comparative Austronesian Dictionary. New
York and Berlin: Mouton, pp. 1008-1149.
APPENDIX A: First Stress Shift and Syllable Reduction Changes in Rejang
Rejang data is provided for three dialects: Musi, Lebong, and Kebanagung. Tables A-1, A-2 and Rules (1)-(15) of this Appendix illustrate the claim that all syllable-reduction changes in Rejang involved the loss of unstressed schwas within the reconstructed pre-Rejang Malay-type stress pattern[15]. (PN = prepenultimate vowel neutralization; DEL = loss of a syllable.)
Pre-Rej PN DEL Musi Lebong Keban. Gloss
A
*ba:qeru beqeRu: beRu belew belaw blew new
*baRa:ni beRa:ni bani biney binay biney brave
*ka-wa:nan kewa:nan kanan kanen kanen kanen right
*qasi:Ra siRa siley silay siley salt
*qapeju: peju pegew n.d. n.d. gall
*qatelu:R teluR tenoa tenoa tenoa egg
*qalimeta:q litaq litea' litea' n.d. leech
*ma-i:Raq me-i:Raq miRaq milea' milea' n.c. red
*ma-a:ñud me-a:ñud mañut monot monot monot drift
*um-u:taq em-u:taq mutaq mutea' mutea' mutah vomit
*in-u:taq en-u:taq nutaq nutea' nutea' nutah (passive)
B
*beReqa:t beRet be'et n.c. behet heavy
*bi:nehiq biniq bénéa' n.d. n.d. seed
*biti:qis beteqi:s betis betis n.d. n.d. leg
*ma-Ruqa:nay meReqa:nay manay[16] -manié -maney -maneé male
*pala:qepaq peleqepa:q pelpaq pelpea' n.d. n.d. palm
*ti:meRaq timaq timea' timea' n.d. tin
*tu:qelaN teqela:N telaN telan telan telan bone
C
*da:qan dan dan n.d. n.d. branch
*emi:s mis mis mis mis sweet
*epa:t pat pat pat pat four
*na:hik naik né' né' nék climb
*pa:qit pait pét pét pét bitter
D.
*lem lem lem lem inside
*la:ud laut laut laut sea
*la:in leyen luyen leyen other
Table A-1: Illustrative Derivations
Table A-2 below illustrates the claim that only prepenultimate vowels underwent rule (1); disyllabic bases were unaffected.
PMP pre-Rej Musi Lebong Keban. Gloss
*daqan da:qan dan dan dan branch
*taqun ta:qun taun taun taun year
*puqun pu:qun pun pun pun tree
*tuqa tu:a tuey tuay tui old
*kahu kahu ko ko ko 2s (sub/obj)
*ni-hu nihu nu nu nu 2s (poss)
*laud la:ud laut laut laut sea
*dahun da:hun dawen dawen dawen leaf
*buhek bu:hek bu' bu' buk hair
*Duha Du:ha duey duay dui two
Table A-2: Disyllables Were Unaffected by Rule (1)
APPENDIX B: Syllable Reduction Changes
(1) Prepenultimate vowels to the left of intervocalic *-q- (but
not *-h- from *S) neutralized as schwa, causing the stress
to shift to the ultimate in the Malay-type pattern. See
Table A-2 above, and consider the contrast between Rejang and
Malay in set A below.
Pre- (1) Rejang Malay GLOSS
Rejang (Musi)
A. *ba:qeRu beqeRu: belew baru new
*tu:qelaN teqela:N telan tulang bone
*biti:qis beteqi:s betis betis calf of leg
*tina:qi teneqi: tenei>teney n.c. stomach
B. *bi:nehiq bénéa' benih seed
*ti:meRaq timea' timah tin
*tu:pelak tula' tolak push
(2) *q and *h (from PAN *S) disappeared, with the exception
that word-final *-q was retained as glottal stop.
*qasi:Ra > *asi:Ra (siley) salt
*hekan > *ekan (kan) fish
*beteqi:s > *betei:s (betis) calf of leg
*bi:nehiq > *bi:neiq (bénéa') seed for planting
*bu:hek > *bu:ek (bu') head hair
*Ru:maq > *Ru:maq (ume:a') house
(3) (Reanalysis) Derived sequence *-ei# became diphthong *-ey > Rejang-Musi ié
(*tina:qi > teneqi: > tenei: > tene:y) > teni:é `stomach'.
(4) Other schwas in derived VV clusters disappeared.
*betei:s > beti:s `calf of leg'
*bu:ek > bu' `head hair'
Note: Rules (5)-(9) constitute the set I have labeled "Blust's Law"
(5) Prepenultimate *a Neutralization (PN *a)
*baRa:ni > *beRa:ni (> ba:ni > bine:y) brave
*ta:kebas > *tekeba:s (> tekba:s > teba:s) clear-cut
*pa:lepaq > *pelepa:q (> pelpa:q > pelpe:a') frond
*ka-wa:nan > *kewa:nan (> ka:nen > kane:n) right
*(q)asi:Ra > *esi:Ra (> si:la > sile:y) salt
*(q)epeju: > peju: (> pege:w) gall
(6) Prepenultimate *#e Deletion (PD *e)
*esi:Ra > si:Ra > si:la (> siley) salt
*epeju: > peju: (> pegew) gall[17]
(7) Schwa Syncope (SS)
*ti:meRaq > *ti:mRaq tin
*tu:pelak > *tu:plak push
*tekeba:s > *tekba:s clear-cut
(8) CC Reduction: -CC- > C (except -lC-) (CR)
Morphological effects:
*ti:mRaq > *ti:maq -CC- remained unreduced
*tu:plak > *tu:lak at morpheme boundaries
*tekba:s > teba:s (t-em-ney, t-en-mew, etc.)
*pelpa:q > pelpe:a'
(9) Prepenultimate *i, *u neutralization (PN *i, *u)
*binatang > benatang animal
*um-inem > *eminem (> méném) drink
*t-um-imbak > t-em-ia' `shoot'
*t-in-imbak > t-en-ia' `be shot'
(10) Derived sequence -ew- disappeared:
*kewa:nan > ka:nan (> kane:n)
*beR-munuq > be-munuq ( > bemonoa')
(11) Derived sequence -eR- disappeared:
*beRa:ni > ba:ni (> bine:y)
*beR-anak[18] > b-anak
(12) All word-initial schwas disappeared (cf. (6).
*eminem > méném drink
*emi:s > mis sweet
*epa:t > pat four
*eka:n > kan fish
*ene:m > num six[19]
(13) Adjacent sequences of like vowels coalesced into a
single vowel
*da:qan > *da:an > dan branch
*pu:qun > *pu:un > pun tree
(14) Derived -ai- clusters collapsed to [é], and one derived -au-
cluster collapsed to [o] in a pronoun.
*pa:qit > *pa:it > pét bitter
*na:hik > *na:ik > nék climb
*kahu > *kau > ko you(2s)
(15) The sequence #niV# reduced to nV (affecting two pronouns).
PMP *h>ø *a>e/V(C) _ #[20] *i>ø Gloss
|
[-stress]
*ni-a nie ne 3s poss.
*ni-hu *niu nu 2s poss.
This completes the (partially) ordered list of changes resulting in the loss of a syllable in Rejang historical phonology.
APPENDIX C: Rejang Changes Associated with the Second Stress Shift
As reported in McGinn (1997), Rejang underwent a SECOND STRESS SHIFT whereby the stress shifted to the final syllable of the base. This change initiated a battery of segmental changes illustrated in (1)-(6) below. Outcomes are shown in the Musi dialect.
(1) Unstressed *a underwent four harmonization patterns:
*tali: > tili: > tiley (open final syllable)
*sapu: > supu: > supew "
*manu:k > monu:k > mono' (closed final syllable)
*langi:t > léngi:t > léngét "
(2) Stressed schwa (*e:) underwent two harmonizations:
*qute:k > oto:' brain
*puse:j > posok navel
*ipe:n > épén tooth
*mine:m > méném drink
(3) Stressed *-i and *-u diphthongized:
*is:i > ise:y contents
*ulu: > ule:w `head'
(4) (Derived) stressed *-e: (schwa) raised to *-i: and
diphthongized following the pattern of (3) above:
*bunge: > bungi: > bunge:y flower
*tue: > tui: > tue:y old
(5) Stressed vowels also diphthongized before liquids and glottal
stop (then final liquids disappeared):
*Rumaq > ume:a' house
*tawaD > tawe:a haggle
*kawil > kéwé:a fishhook
*dengeR > tengo:a hear
(6) Single phonemes developed from intervocalic sequences of
(homorganic) nasal + obstruent:
*sempit > sepit [spit] narrow
*tungked > tokot [to.ko:t] staff, cane
*timbak > tia' [ti.a:'] shoot
*induk > indo' [ind.o:'] mother (of animals)
APPENDIX D: Evidence for Reconstructing a Malay-Type Stress
Pattern for Pre-Rejang
Two arguments are presented here to justify reconstructing a Malay-type stress pattern[21] for pre-Rejang. (For more see McGinn 1997). First, the reconstruction is necessary to capture the regularity of a number of sound changes, the most important of which is (1) below.
(1) *a > e /-V(C) (C)# :where -C = [-velar]
|
[-stress]
This rule says that unstressed *a changed to schwa in disyllables except when followed by a velar (*-k, *-q, *-ng). Rule (1) must be ordered after the set of syllable-reduction changes shown in Appendix A. Consider the following evidence.
(2)
A. CVCVC Disyllables *bu:lat > *bu:let
*pa:nas > *pa:nes
*ta:ngan > *ta:ngen
*qu:Zan > *u:jen
B. CVCVC Oxytones *ta:kebas > teba:s (not tebes*)
*tu:qelaN > tela:n (not telen*)
*teba:ng > teba:ng (not tebeng*
C. CVC Monosyllables *heka:n > kan (not ken*)
*qa:yam > yam (not yem*)
*epa:t > pat (not pet*)
Notice that *a in monosyllables (set C) and the second syllable of `oxytone' disyllables (set B) were unaffected by rule (1), as predicted, because rule (1) affected unstressed vowels. The reconstructed Malay-type stress pattern accounts for these data and establishes their regularity in relation to the rule (sound change).
Second, consider the following generalizations that can be adduced from the Rejang changes illustrated in Appendix B and especially the harmonization rules in Appendix C.
(3) a) PMP *a was less stable than *e (schwa) in Rejang[22].
b) The changes affecting PMP simple vowels caused the
lexicon to split based on the vocalic feature [low]:
System A System B
[-low] [+low]
i u e
é e o a
At first glance (3a) seems `unnatural'; if the low central vowel [a] represents the ideal (=most sonorous) vowel on the sonority hierarchy, and if schwa is the least sonorous, then all things being equal [a] should be a more stable vowel than schwa. The reconstructed stress pattern resolves the paradox: all unstable *a's and stable schwas were in unstressed positions. Unstressed *a changed (harmonized) in conformity with System A; and unstressed *e (schwa) resisted change only in the penult position of disyllables, in words that conformed with Systems A and B. In contrast, unstressed penult schwas were maximally unstable in trisyllables (they disappeared); and stressed schwas underwent harmonization (see rule (2) of Appendix C).
[1]The singulary term `drift' means little more than `regular sound changes' plus the further suggestion that these tend to be directional. See section 6 for discussion.
[2]As for Malay's multi-purpose derivational suffix -an, it is uncertain whether -an is a retention from PAN or an innovation similar to -kan.
[3]The Malay passive prefix /di-/ presumably derives from PAN variant *ni- via denasalization. See Teeuw (1959) for extensive discussion.
[4]What it shifted from remains unknown; but that the stress pattern of PAN/PMP was not Malay-type is taken for granted here. See Ross (1992), Wolff (1991) and Zorc (1978) for arguments and discussion.
[5]The exception is found in in Malay suffixes -kan and -i, which can receive the stress under the influence of enclitic pronoun /nya/: bicára > bicará-kan > bicara-kán-nya. See Cohn and McCarthy (1994:MS).
[6] Presumably these had stress on the ultimate in the Malay-type pattern; thus: CeCV:C.
[7] CeCVC shapes are oxytone (CeCV:C) in the Malay-type stress system.
[8] Although rare to nonexistent in PMP, conceivably *m- initial CeCVC bases should also be included here.
[9] i.e. the chage affected CVCVC bases as well as CeCVC bases.
[10]/be-/ likely derives from PAN/PMP *maR-, and /pe-/ from PAN/PMP *paR-. (See Teeuw 1959:141 and Wolff 1981:84 for discussion of comparable Malay affixes).
[11]Compare Malay (tidak) peduli `(don't) care' = Rejang peduley `concern' (noun), which yields a base that can be infixed:
d-em-uley `show concern'.
[12]See Coady and McGinn (1982) for a synchronic description of the barred nasals.
[13]In other words, penult stress extends beyond the base to encompass the phonological word in Malay.
[14]The exception is that Rejang tolerates intervocalic consonant clusters when the second element is /l/ in bases and complex formations: kemelbea' `very; extremely' (based on -em-, ke- and lebea' `more', cf. Malay /lebih/); /pelpea'/ `midrib of coconut frond' (<> **aju > **agu > **ugu > **ugew (cf. *sapu > supew `broom').
[18]I assume pre-Rejang *beR- derives from PAN *maR- (Teeuw 1959:141; Wolff 1981:84).
[19]Note: *e > /u/ in /num/ is unexplained.
[20]This is a special application of rule C-(1) in Appendix C.
[21]In a Malay-type pattern the stress is on the ultimate when the penult is schwa; otherwise on the penult.
[22]According to Blust (1984) and McGinn (1997), PMP *e split into seven phonemes in Rejang and PMP *a split into nine.
source : rejang-lebong.blogspot.com
Tidak ada komentar:
Posting Komentar